Confession Of A Guilt Essay

Guilt, Suffering, Confession And Redemption In Crime And Punishment

Guilt, Suffering, Confession and Redemption in Crime and Punishment      


    "You keep lying!" screamed Raskolnikov, no longer able to restrain himself. "You're lying, you damned clown!" And he flung himself on Porfiry, who retired to the doorway, but without a trace of panic. "I understand everything, everything!" He approached Porfiry. "You're lying and taunting me so Ill give myself away-" "You can't give yourself away any more than you have already, Rodion Romanovich, old man. Why, you've gone into a state. Don't shout, I'll call my men, sir!" (Dostoyevsky, 34)

No humane person with any values is able to commit a heinous crime without some feeling of guilt or remorse afterwards. Slowly, this guilt festers and eats away at one's conscience until the point of escape, reached by confession, thus leading to salvation. Throughout Dostoyevsky's Crime and. Punishment the main character, Raskolnikov is stricken with guilt and suffering that eventually lead to his confession and redemption motivated by many forces.

Crime and Punishment is the story of a young "intellect", Raskolnikov, who develops a superman theory. In his hypothesis, he felt that certain men were extraordinary and could commit unethical acts without punishment or a guilty conscience. In his case, he wanted to rid the earth of a parasite through the vicious slaying of an old pawnbroker, Alyona, and her sister, Lizaveta, in order to gain money so that he could continue his studies and to see if he was truly extraordinary. Was he truly the Napoleon that he thought he was? Could he walk over people with no regard for their feelings or sufferings as Napoleon had? (Literary Criticism, 68) "He is obviously no superman or Napoleon, but didn't get enough fresh air to his cubicles." (Timoney)

Raskolnikov Dostoyevsky?

It may be seen among many critics that Dostoyevsky could have directly related to his character, Raskolnikov. Both of them had experienced sufferings beyond what one can imagine. Dostoyevsky felt that suffering gives one the chance of puri6cation and transformation. Through his many ordeals of suffering, ranging from the near death of his sick children or his epileptic seizures, to his early imprisonment and exile to Siberia, Dostoyevsky felt that he eventually reached the point of happiness "The way of salvation is the way of suffering" could often be heard mumbled from Dostoyevsky's mouth. For these reasons, critics believe that Raskolnikov may have been a direct symbol of Dostoyevsky and his suffering. (Kjetsaa, 346-349)

Dostoyevsky most likely modeled Crime and Punishment after his own experiences. Since the two men's lives had striking similarities, many people believed that was evidence to prove their thinking. As Raskolnikov was overcome with tremendous suffering, sent to Siberia, and fell madly in love with a beautiful woman; so did Dostoyevsky within his lifetime. "I do believe that the character (Raskolnikov) was the epitome of...

Loading: Checking Spelling

0%

Read more

Religious Influences in the Path for Redemption in Crime and Punishment

1511 words - 6 pages With the prominent focus in Crime and Punishment by Fyodor Dostoevsky being the path for redemption and the search for hope, a connection can be made with the religious influences throughout the novel. Such religious influences throughout the Christian faith can most prominently be seen in how the characters such as Raskolnikov develop. Needing a vessel to communicate and push these religious influences onto a struggling and tormented...

Free Essays on Crime and Punishment - Suffering, Death, and Resurrection

568 words - 2 pages Crime and Punishment -  Suffering, Death, and Resurrection Fyodor Dostoevsky wrote the novel, Crime and Punishment, during a turbulent time in Russian history. Yet his work will speaks to any age. Dostoevsky  wrote to warn against what he considered the negative effects of the trend of nihilism and rational egoism. He advances this objective by employing themes of suffering, resurrection, and death--all of these currents running through a...

Crime and Punishment report - Although suffering appears to be hopeless and unpromising it does indeed ennoble character.

898 words - 4 pages Elena Smukler Crime and PunishmentHe remained alone in a world that only he understood. He sat in a room like a spider, days went without food, and he lay in the darkness without purpose. He rested still thinking, his mind swarming with strange dreams of which he could not even begin to describe. How could he become a man daring enough to stoop and pick up the power, for in the eyes of men whomever is daring enough to do so is powerful...

Crime And Punishment In America

893 words - 4 pages Crime is something wrongfully done against another person, place, or thing. Too much of the wrong thing is being done in the fight against "vice and sin". Religion impacts laws in various ways, laws are based on religion and our religion instills in us, our morals. Since the morals of the community are what the laws are based on in turn religion has a great impact on the laws. Freedom for women revolutionized the employment for women, it...

Seeking Atonement in Crime and Punishment

660 words - 3 pages Seeking Atonement in Crime and Punishment   Raskolnikov, the protagonist of Dostoyevsky's Crime and Punishment was a complicated man who committed a crime. Raskolnikov murdered a woman who was a plague to mankind, especially the poor of Russia. In the chilling process however, he also murdered her younger sister, Lisaveta. To be purified, he drives himself through much agony. Not until the closing of the novel did he realize he must...

Crime and Punishment

1321 words - 5 pages Crime and Punishment In his book “Crime and Punishment”, Dostoevsky explores the path of Raskolnikov who has many problems and obstacles throughout his life. He commits murder and is faced with the long and mentally extremely painful journey of seeking redemption. Raskolnikov believes that by a law of nature men have been “somewhat arbitrarily” divided into two groups of “ordinary” and “extraordinary”. Raskolnikov believes that the duty of...

Crime and Punishment

1311 words - 5 pages In his book "Crime and Punishment", Dostoevsky explores the path of Raskolnikov who has many problems and obstacles throughout his life. He commits murder and is faced with the long and mentally extremely painful journey of seeking redemption.Raskolnikov believes that by a law of nature men have been...

Harry Potter and Kenneth Burke’s Guilt Redemption Cycle

3201 words - 13 pages Introduction There are probably few people in this world who can deny the feeling of guilt. Some call that sinking feeling in the pit of the stomach or that nagging voice in their head their conscious speaking to them. Whatever people want to call this feeling, few can deny never feeling it. Is it this connection of the feeling guilt and the need to alleviate it that attracts people so much to characters who have imperfections? Is it this...

Imperfect Conscience in Dostoevsky's Crime and Punishment

566 words - 2 pages Crime and Punishment:  Imperfect Conscience               A highly educated individual, avoiding the hardships of society while pondering the possibility of great wealth, Raskolnikov, in Fyodor Dostoevsky's "Crime and Punishment," frustrated with his immoral actions, suffers from an abrupt physical and mental breakdown after brutally mutilating a wicked pawnbroker. After this soul-scarring incident, the initial feelings of success in...

Crime and Punishment in the U.S.

1070 words - 4 pages Crime and Punishment in the United States In the Bible, crime is called sin and harsh punishments are prescribed for committing them. In our society, crime is defined as a violation of criminal law, so no matter how heinous an act might be it is not a crime unless the criminal law has listed it and provided a punishment for it (Coleman, 322). There are many criminal laws on the books today that we might consider ridiculous, but at some...

Psychoanalysis in Dostoevsky's Crime and Punishment

2761 words - 11 pages Analyzing the mind of a sociopath has been one of the most important tasks that psychoanalysts face today. The more they know and understand the complexities of the disturbed, the more they hope to find treatments and eventually a cure for the illness that they believe can cause the ultimate violent criminal. Perhaps Dostoevsky himself wanted to weigh in on the mind of the sociopath and the journey toward their violent lives. Due to his...

Like a whole bunch of people, I recently got caught up in the new Netflix series, Making a Murderer. It was a fascinating–and frustrating–glimpse into police dynamics, judicial procedure, and human psychology. I’ve written about psychology and the law elsewhere on this blog, and I’m not here to make any particular claims about the series’ subject, Steven Avery’s, guilt or innocence, but one plotline speaks to an interesting psychological phenomenon: the psychology of false confessions.

In the series, Brendan Dassey endures hours of interrogation, without his lawyer present, before finally confessing to rape and murder along with his uncle. The filmmakers compellingly argue that there are serious reasons to believe that Dassey’s confession was coerced (i.e., he never did anything but said he did). In the courtroom proceedings, however, the prosecutor argues that “innocent people don’t confess.” Things don’t seem to have changed much since 1923 when John Henry Wigmore wrote that false confessions were “scarcely conceivable.”

But could that be true? Do innocent people ever confess–and why? It might seem unreasonable to believe that an innocent person would confess to a crime that he or she didn’t commit, but there are plenty of cases in which people did just that.

The Reality of False Confessions

In his excellent book, Unfair: The New Science of Criminal Injustice, Adam Benforado writes, “[f]alse confessions and incriminating statements are the leading contributors to wrongful homicide convictions, present in over 60 percent of the known DNA murder-exoneration cases in the United States.”

Of course, it’s hard to collect any reasonable statistics about the rates of false confessions that haven’t been later proven false. How could we possibly know they happened? That aside, we can at least say that they do sometimes happen. Let’s take it all the way back to the Salem Witch Trials of 1672 in which about 50 women confessed to being witches, according to historical accounts.

Since then, there have been many provocative cases of people who have confessed to crimes they did not commit, only to be found false later through other evidence (e.g., DNA). There’s Damon Thibodeaux who, after a 9-hour interrogation, gave a recorded confession to murdering his cousin. There’s Juan Rivera, who signed a written confession to the rape and murder of an 11-year-old girl. There’s Phillip Bivens who spent 30 years in prison, having confessed to rape and murder, before being exonerated with DNA evidence in 2010. All three of these people were eventually found innocent, making their initial confessions pretty perplexing. For more examples, see the comprehensive directory at The Innocence Project.

Why it Matters: Confession are Powerful

It’s not just that innocent people confess and the error is quickly discovered. As you can see in the above cases, people end up going to jail–often for a very long time–before their confession is found faulty (if it ever is). The reason is simply that juries are convinced by confessions. After all, if someone admits to murdering someone, surely he understands that this puts him in jail for a very long time, and why would someone do that unless he was guilty?

We tend to have a hard time believing that innocent people confess to crimes, and in fact, classic social psychological research has documented why we’re resistant to the idea that confessions can be coerced out of innocent people.

Maybe you’ve heard of the fundamental attribution error. The gist of it is that we’re pretty hesitant to say that external forces influence other people’s behaviors. If someone is rude to us, for example, we’re more likely to chalk it up to the person’s salty personality than we are to consider that the person is just having a bad day and would otherwise be gracious and polite.

In one classic study, researchers gave people essays that argued in favor of or against Fidel Castro. After reading the essays, everyone made a guess about the author’s true feelings about Castro. Naturally, people who read a pro-Castro essay figured the author was a Castro supporter, and people who read an anti-Castro essay figured the author was anti-Castro. So far, this isn’t that unreasonable.

But sometimes, when the researchers gave people that article, they told them that the author had no choice about which position he took. He was assigned to take a particular position and had no control over it. Rationally, this should mean that the readers wouldn’t attribute the essay’s position to the author’s true opinion. However, even when they knew that the author was forced to write an essay with a particular conclusion, they still assumed that the essay’s position matched the author’s true opinion.

So relating this back to false confessions, it might be the case that even when there’s considerable doubt about the truth of a person’s confession, a jury may still find truth in that confession via the same attribution error.

Why Would an Innocent Person Confess?

Although it’s clear it happens and that it can have important consequences, you might still be wondering, “but why!?” After all, when you think about it, do you think you would confess to a murder you didn’t commit? Of course not! So how could it happen?

One reason is that by the end of a lengthy interrogation, people just want to be done with it. Whereas ordinary police interrogations usually last less than 2 hours, research on documented false confessions has found that interrogations that produced false confessions lasted 16.3 hours, on average. That’s a long time to be questioned as an innocent person! I’ll talk more in a bit about why these interrogations might go on so long.

On the one hand, these lengthy interrogations obviously produce fatigue and sleep deprivation, which have been shown to impair decision-making and increase suggestibility. In addition, other experimental research has found that innocent people who resist falsely confessing show greater sympathetic nervous system activation–signs of physiological stress–than innocent or guilty confessors.

On the other hand, after such a long process, suspects are often reminded that if they just confessed, they could end the whole unpleasant experience and go home. In this way, maintaining your statement of innocence is an act of self-control! In general, people often prefer things that will provide immediate benefits, compared to having to suffer now and reap rewards later. It’s like the conflict between eating a delicious slice of pie right now or abstaining from dessert to lose weight in the future.

In an interrogation room, it’s a conflict between getting relief now (confessing) and avoiding jail in the future (maintaining innocence). This still might seem crazy, but after more than 10 hours of badgering, the ability to make it all stop could be too tempting to resist.

There’s another conflict at play, too. Going to court can be a long, drawn-out process, and it’s one that we likely wouldn’t look forward to (especially when it means people will be pointing the finger at us the whole time and that our punishment would be steeper if found guilty). Sometimes, it can just seem easier to admit to a crime than to endure the legal proceedings and risk a worse fate.

In fact, one study simulated these very conditions. Researchers took unsuspecting students and gave them a test of logic puzzles to complete in the same room as another student. Although students diligently did their own work, the researchers later accused these innocent participants of cheating on the test. The students then had a choice: admit to cheating and lose the study participation reward they would have gotten or have the issue brought before the university’s “Academic Review Board” where students are found guilty 90% of the time. The latter option also came with mandatory ethics training if found guilty.

The results are revealing: 56% of innocent students chose to admit guilt for a smaller punishment instead of risk a drawn out ethics case review. These are objectively innocent students who are choosing to admit guilt for fear of a risky trial.

Some People are More Likely to Falsely Confess

It’s worth noting that there are two groups that are disproportionately likely to give false confessions: young people and cognitively impaired people. Brendan Dassey belonged to both of these groups.

Some studies have simply asked people of various ages whether they would confess to a crime that they did not commit. Researchers tell them a story about someone faced with such a decision and then ask what the correct choice would be. In one study, even after controlling for differences in measured intelligence, 13 – 15 year-old boys were more likely to choose false confession than 16 – 18 year-olds.

There is also a disproportionate amount of false confessions that come from cognitively impaired individuals. There could be many reasons for this, one of which is that this population tends to respond “yes” to a range of questions to which an affirmative response is not appropriate.

In both of these populations, false confessions might also be especially likely because of a desire to gain the approval of adults and people of authority. Indeed, overall, people who falsely confess tend to be more compliant people in general.

Are the Police to be Blamed?

It would be bold of me to say that police want to get innocent people to confess to crimes. However, I think that a social psychological perspective reveals some definite flaws in common interrogation practices.

The first is that investigators often enter an interrogation with the expectation that the person is guilty. The goal, then, is not to uncover truth–it’s to extract a confession. Social psychology research is filled with warnings against strong expectations. When we believe something is true, we’re biased to find confirmation of that. We ask leading questions, we interpret ambiguous behavior in ways that align with our expectation, we adopt mannerisms that help elicit the response we expect from others, etc.

In his book, Benforado reviews an experiment with mock interrogators which found that “participants who started off with a guilt mindset were more than 20 percent more likely to confirm guilt than those who started off with an innocence mindset.

Similarly, some of the techniques used by interrogators have been shown to produce false confessions, just as they produce true confessions. These techniques of “minimization” and “maximization” involve trivializing the offense (“You didn’t mean to hurt anyone!”) and offering a deal (“If you give us what we want, we’ll make sure things get settled quickly”). One study that recreated the conditions of interrogating truly guilty and truly innocent participants found that these techniques were successful in producing confessions from innocent participants.

The Psychology of False Confessions and Implanted Memories

Implanting memories is not just the stuff of Inception. I want to wrap up by noting that even after considering all of these forces that elicit false confessions, one of the scary outcomes is that the suspect may begin to believe the confession him or herself.

False memorieshave been a common topic of research in psychology and the law. Simply changing the way a question is asked can succeed in influencing a person’s memory of an actual event. There are also issues pertaining to eyewitness identification and faulty memory. For now, though, we’ll just focus on false confessions and their effect on memory.

The real problem is that not only do innocent people sometimes confess due to external pressures, but they can also come to believe that they committed the crime. As one example, a 14-year-old boy was accused of stabbing his sister. Although he strongly denied these claims (and was later actually found innocent), by the end of his extensive questioning he transformed, saying “I’m not sure how I did it. All I know is I did it.”

Some laboratory research has shown how this can happen to unsuspecting members of the public. In one study, researchers accused student participants of breaking a computer by using the wrong keys. In truth, all of the participants were innocent and initially denied the accusation. By using common interrogation tactics, however, the procedure ended with 69% of innocent participants signing a confession. Furthermore, 28% had actually come to believe that they broke the computer, and 9% had even come up with additional details (that didn’t happen) to support their new belief.

So, Do Innocent People Confess?

I think the answer is a pretty clear “yes.” The experience of being interrogated by people who are trained to extract confessions and who believe the suspect to be guilty creates a host of powerful psychological influences that can break down a truly innocent person. Moreover, another set of psychological states makes false confessions believable enough to send people to prison.

Now I’m not saying that every confession is false, nor am I necessarily saying that Brendan Dassey’s confession was false. The real point here is that we really ought to think carefully about how a confession was obtained, and we shouldn’t be so quick to assume that false confessions never happen. I’d encourage you to check out the website of The Innocence Project for more on how these individuals finally find justice and what changes could be implemented to reduce the rate of coerced confessions.

Also, I haven’t covered every detail of the psychology of false confessions, so join in on the discussion and point out additional considerations. Oh, and if you haven’t watched Making a Murderer, it’s about time! Especially if you’re as interested in human psychology as I am.


Feature Image: “Interrogation” by Paul Kehrer –  Licensed under Creative Commons CC BY 2.0 via Flickr, cropped

Related

Categories BlogTags Biases, deception, memory, psychology and law

0 thoughts on “Confession Of A Guilt Essay

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *